Critical thinking (rational thinking) is good
thinking; it involves forming appropriate goals, goal optimization and forming evidence
based beliefs. Two common myths associated with critical thinking are emotion prevents
critical thinking and critical thinking is synonymous with formal logic.
Emotions may enhance instrumental rationality, or
they may impede it. Emotions provide an approximation of the correct
response. If more accuracy than that is
required, then a more precise type of analytic cognition will be required
(Stanovich, 2009) It is possible to rely
too much on the emotions. We can base
responses on an approximation when what is really needed is a more precise type
of analytic thought. More often than not,
processes of emotional regulation enhance rational thinking and behavior....
Myths About Rationality
Although many people feel (mistakenly or not) that
they could do without the ability to solve textbook logic problems (which is
why the caricatured view of rationality works to undercut its status),
virtually no person wishes to eschew epistemic rationality and instrumental
rationality, properly defined. Virtually all people want their beliefs to
be in some correspondence with reality, and they also want to act to maximize
the achievement of their goals. Psychologist Ken Manktelow has emphasized
the practicality of both types of rationality by noting that they concern two
critical things: What is true and what to do.....
Formal Logic
Formal logic consists of different categories. It is
concerned with evaluation of argument, often involving what are presumed to be true premises that lead to a conclusion, that
may or may not be valid. Logic isn't a complete system of thinking, as it is
incomplete in evidence search; what matters is the construction of the argument.
Categories of formal logic include: propositional, categorical, predicate and
modal (Baron, 2008). Baron provides a discussion on logic and its various forms
in his book - Thinking And Deciding 4th
Edition (Baron, 2008).
Critical Thinking
Educators, so-called
evidence based practitioners and researchers often pay lip service to the idea of teaching
“critical thinking”. But, when asked to
define “critical thinking” answers are often weak and sometimes so ambiguous
they are virtually worthless. Common
responses when asked to define critical
thinking includes, “teaching them how to think”, “teaching them formal logic”, or “teaching
them how to solve problems”. They
already know how to think; if they have a normal
functioning working memory. When they are awake they are thinking about something.
Logic is only a portion of what is needed to increase critical thinking,
and teaching them how to solve problems is an ambiguous answer, that is context specific. Stanovich argues, “that the superordinate
goal we are actually trying to foster is that of rationality” (Stanovich, 2010,
p.198). Critical thinking, as it is commonly
referred, is a weak concept that lacks converging evidence. And, if measured at
all, it is often measured in a subjective way, not susceptible to replication. Cognitive
science, conceptualizes clearly and provides an array of measures of rational
thinking tasks. The first comprehensive assessment was made public in 2016.